Tuesday, July 1, 2008

Laughing out loud: scientists have made new discoveries about laughter



The Science of Laughs

Scanning brains and eavesdropping on chimps, researchers are figuring out why we chuckle, guffaw and crack up. Hint: it isn’t funny.

By Sharon Begley
NEWSWEEK

      October 9 issue: The man was known around town as a
chronic borrower, always asking the neighbors to lend him one
tool or another. So when he strolled over to Mr. Green one
Sunday and asked, “Say, are you using your lawn mower today?”
Green had a ready answer. “Yes, I am,” he said emphatically.
         TO WHICH THE would-be borrower replied, "Great! Then
since you won't need your golf clubs, I'll just borrow those."

        If you are like most people, that punch line triggered
at least one short exhalation of breath chopped into staccato
segments lasting about one fifteenth of a second each and spaced
one fifth of a second apart. In other words, laughter. Exactly
why humor causes this acoustic response (rather than, say, sweaty
palms or any other physiological reaction) has been lost in the
mists of evolution. Thinkers from Plato and Aristotle to Kant,
Darwin and Freud have tried to fathom laughter (Plato feared it
would disrupt the state, and Kant theorized that it arises when
what you expect to happen doesn't, which is why surprising punch
lines work), but they've been long on philosophy and short on
science. Lately, though, an intrepid band of researchers has
been trying to remedy that. With techniques like MRI brain scans
to probe why people cannot tickle themselves into paroxysms of
laughter, they are tackling "one of the last great unsolved
problems in human behavior," says neuroscientist Robert Provine
of the University of Maryland, whose book, "Laughter: A
Scientific Investigation," will be published next week. And no,
he's not referring to why guys are always walking into bars with
ducks under their arms.

 "Laughter," says Provine, "is a probe into such fundamental
questions as why humans can speak but other apes can't."
ROBERT PROVINE
University of Maryland

           To investigate the roots of laughter, scientists have
turned to our primate cousins. If you tickle a chimp (carefully;
and preferably a baby), it will likely laugh, but the sound
doesn_t resemble human laughter so much as it does panting, with
one sound per inhale and exhale. Provine realized that the
reason chimps cannot emit a string of "ho ho ho's" is that they
cannot make more than a single sound when they exhale or inhale.
Humans, in contrast, can chop up a single exhalation into
multiple bursts of "ha ha ha" or words. (Speech results from
chopping up an exhalation into separate sounds.) "Laughter,"
says Provine, "is a probe into such fundamental questions as why
humans can speak but other apes can't." No humanlike laughing,
no speaking.

        The fact that chimps pant while playing suggests that
laughter evolved from the heavy breathing that accompanies
something like playful wrestling. Ritualized
panting laughter then might have come to represent the playful
activity itself, signaling "I'm enjoying this." For there is no
question that human laughter is a social behavior. (You would
have laughed more at the borrow-the-mower joke if you had heard
it read aloud while in a group, rather than reading it silently
and alone.)

        But what do titters communicate? Some clues come from
Provine's collection of 1,200 "laugh episodes," from
eavesdropping in public places. He finds that speakers laugh
more than listeners, and women laugh at men more than vice
versa. (Laughing behind their backs doesn't count.) Laughter
seems to signal an attempt to ingratiate oneself: in India,
notes Provine, men of lower castes giggle when addressing men of
higher castes, but never the other way round. People in power
seldom giggle. More evidence that laughing has less to do with
humor than with social signals is that, in Provine's 1,200
samples, by far the remarks that most often elicited laughter
were of the _it was nice meeting you, too" or "I know" variety.
In other words, witless. "Laughter is only rarely a response to
jokes," says Provine. "It is, instead, the quintessential human
social signal. It solidifies relationships and pulls people into
the fold." And not only for the good. The Columbine killers
laughed as they shot, demonstrating solidarity with each other
and, needless to say, not their targets. Evil laughter is no
oxymoron.

        Laughter is contagious, for reasons that remain pretty
murky, but while you can catch it from others you can_t induce
it in yourself. Forced laughter sounds, well, forced. And
tickling yourself is a non-starter. There have been all sorts of
theories about why, but recent experiments led by Sarah-Jayne
Blakemore of University College, London, may have finally
revealed the answer. The neuroscientists used functional MRI to
(noninvasively) peer into the brains of volunteers who either
tickled themselves or were tickled by a robotic tickler.

The fMRI detected more neuronal activity in the somatosensory
cortex, the part of the brain that registers touch, when people
were tickled than when they tickled themselves. The reason seems
to be that when you move your fingers to, say, tickle your rib
cage, the cerebellum, which coordinates complex movements,
predicts what that will feel like and sends out a signal to
cancel the sensory response much as the military can jam an
enemy radio transmission if it knows the frequency ahead of
time. But if the tickle comes from someone else, and so is a
surprise, the cerebellum can't block it. Self-tickling thus
seems to make neurons fire in a way that blocks activation of
the part of the brain that ordinarily processes touch.

        Laughter seems intimately entwined with our physiology.
It blocks a neural reflex that regulates muscle tone, proving
that _going weak with laughter_ is more than a metaphor. Tumors
or lesions of the brain's hypothalamus, which regulates basic
processes like respiration, can cause bouts of uncontrolled
laughter. And although laughter seems to have evolved because of
the message it sends to others, it may have a therapeutic effect
on the laugher, too: it can raise heart rate as much as aerobic
exercise, lessen the perception of pain and increase tolerance
of discomfort. While the scientists work out how a good chuckle
does all this, have you heard the one about the priest, the
minister and the rabbi... ?

       ) 2000 Newsweek, Inc.



No comments:

Post a Comment